Monografias.com > Psicología
Descargar Imprimir Comentar Ver trabajos relacionados

Metacognition and its development



     

    1. Metacognitive skills and
      metacognitive knowledge
    2. The emergence of conscious
      control
    3. The fallibility of metacognitive
      knowledge
    4. Metacognitive
      strategies
    5. Some practical
      implications
    6. Assessing students'
      self-esteem
    7. Enhancing students'
      self-esteem
    8. Peer teaching and peer
      assessment
    9. Conclusion
    10. References

    Being aware of our thinking as we perform a specific task
    and then using this awareness to control what we
    are doing is commonly known in thinking skills literature as
    "metacognition". More recently, the term "metacognitive approach"
    has been applied to strategy training aimed at teaching EFL
    students consciously to plan, monitor and
    evaluate their own learning and to analyse the different stages
    of a task in order to choose appropriate problem-solving
    strategies (see Robbins 2002). The purpose of this article is to
    provide some theoretical insights into the nature of
    metacognition and to outline additional ways of supporting
    students' metacognitive development.

     Metacognitive skills and metacognitive
    knowledge

     A clear distinction is generally
    made between metacognitive skills and metacognitive knowledge.
    Metacognitive skills develop initially out of self-correcting
    activities in domain-specific learning (Bruner 1986 quoted in Von
    Wright 1992; 64) as children gradually learn to anticipate chains
    of events and compare alternative procedures or mentally correct
    an action plan before acting. Although these actions are often
    intentional – i.e. purposeful and directed towards conscious
    goals – (Von Wright 1992:61), most children nevertheless have
    difficulty in reflecting on their own intentions and seeing their
    goals as choices that exist among a number of alternative goals.
    Metacognitive skills improve task performance, but the choice of
    task remains largely predetermined by unconscious (or external)
    factors. Voluntary action depends on metacognitive knowledge,
    which results from introspection or
    self-reflection.

      The
    emergence of conscious control

     In order to understand how people
    come to gain control over their actions, we need to understand
    how self-knowledge and the ability to reflect on one's own
    behaviour emerge. It is here that computer-based models of
    cognition, which support much work on cognitive strategies, break
    down (since computers cannot be said to be 'conscious" of what
    they do) and that we must turn to social constructivist accounts
    of cognitive and emotional development for a theoretical
    explanation. Social constructivism starts from the notion that
    individual minds are constructed out of social interactions and
    social meanings. We shall return to the practical implications of
    this point later.

     Vygotsky's (1978) theory of cognitive
    development is well known. Briefly, it states that the L1
    linguistic system is at the root of all higher cognitive
    functions. Firstly, language frees the child from the
    stimulus-bound stage of natural perception. By using verbal
    labelling, the child singles out separate elements and forms "new
    (artificially introduced and dynamic) structural centres" which
    can be re-synthesised into new concepts (1978:32). Later,
    language acts as a cognitive barrier in problem solving,
    mediating between the presentation of the task and the child's
    final response. (By contrast, children with so-called 'attention
    deficit disorder', or ADD, seem to possess little ability to
    delay their responses). In short, problem solving is first
    effected through "ego-centric speech" (the child talks to himself
    or herself) and later, around the age of five, this is replaced
    by inner speech (reflections) (1986:30). Once egocentric speech
    has become thus internalised, the child is able to focus
    consciously on cognitive processes such as memory and to bring
    them under increasingly greater conscious control
    (1986:170).

     However, as Von Wright (1992:61) points out, a
    crucial step towards greater expertise in self-reflection is the
    development of the concept of self. The concept of self is a
    social construct that we acquire by being treated as a self by
    others. In G H Mead's (1934) words: "self-consciousness involves
    the individual's becoming an object to himself by taking the
    attitudes of other individuals towards himself within an
    organised setting of social relationships, and … unless the
    individual had thus become an object to himself, he would not be
    self-conscious or have a self at all" (quoted in Von Wright
    1992:61). This suggests that individuals with a poorly developed
    or confused self-concept will lack insight into their own
    intentions, motives and intellectual functions, and that
    development of metacognitive awareness in later life may
    ultimately depend on early social conditioning. My own
    (unpublished) replication study based on Rosenberg (1979) found
    that self-esteem, rather than age, determined teenagers' and
    young adults' ability to focus on their psychological "inner
    worlds", set realistic goals outside the classroom, follow them
    through, evaluate the results and learn from their
    mistakes.

     The
    fallibility of metacognitive knowledge

     Conventional analyses
    usually divide metacognitive knowledge into knowledge concerning
    person, task and strategy variables (Von
    Wright 1992:64). Thus, Marzano et al (1988) list the various
    types of knowledge that are important to metacognition as: (a)
    executive control, which evaluates current state of knowledge;
    (b) declarative knowledge, which is being conscious of the facts
    surrounding a situation; (c) conditional knowledge which
    describes why a strategy works; (d) procedural knowledge, which
    has to do with various actions performed in a task. However,
    knowing when, how and why to use a particular strategy in an
    objective, factual sense does not guarantee that it will be used.
    This knowledge only counts as metacognitive knowledge when it is
    spontaneously integrated with awareness of our thinking on a
    specific task and when we use this awareness to control what we
    are doing (cited in Harrison 1991:37).

     The value of Von Wright's emphasis on
    self-knowledge, I believe, is that it emphasises the subjective
    basis of metacognitive knowledge. Metacognitive knowledge
    includes conscious knowledge of one's actions, intentions and
    motives, and also of one's intellectual functions. The latter
    "creates conditions for a wider application of specific
    competences and learned rules" (Von Wright 1992:62) by
    integrating information which previously belonged to separate
    cognitive systems (transfer of learning). But, like any other
    type of self-knowledge, it is fallible.

      Metacognitive
    strategies

     How, then, do
    metacognitive strategies such as planning, monitoring and
    evaluating one's own learning evolve? According to Vygotsky
    (1968:168) " in order to subject a function to intellectual and
    volitional control, we must first possess it". In other words,
    self-reflection will develop first as a skill before it can be
    used as a series of consciously controlled strategies. We have
    already noted the role played by language and social
    relationships in the emergence of these processes. The emphasis
    on social interaction as a condition for the training of
    reflective skills is today shared by most approaches to
    instruction (Von Wright 1991:66). Reciprocal (peer) teaching, for
    example, forces the "teacher" to use a whole series of
    metacognitive processes such as determining what the learner
    already knows, deciding what is to be taught/learnt and how;
    monitoring comprehension and evaluating the outcome in terms of
    increased comprehension, which in turn encourage the "teacher" to
    reflect upon his or her own thinking processes (ibid). In social
    constructivist terms, metacognitive processes begin as social
    processes and gradually become
    "internalised".

     Some
    practical implications

     The effective use of
    metacognitive strategies is one of the primary differences
    between more and less able learners and students need to be
    taught such strategies through direct instruction, modelling, and
    practice. Robbins has already provided an excellent bibliography
    for the CALLA approach to strategy training in SHARE 90. Perhaps
    the main implication of this article is that instruction is more
    likely to produce permanent results in students with (1) high
    self-esteem (the basis of accurate metacognitive knowledge) and
    (2) extensive experience of peer teaching and assessment
    (resulting in a broader range of metacognitive
    skills).

     Assessing students'
    self-esteem

     Recent empirical
    research in developmental and educational psychology strongly
    supports a multifaceted view of self-concept, which distinguishes
    academic self-concept from physical self-concept, and so on. The
    clearest example of measures based on this view is Marsh's (1992)
    "Self-Description Questionnaire I, II, or III" for ages seven to
    young adult. Other widely used measures, such as Fitts' (1991)
    "Tennessee Self Concept Scale", stress the
    distinctiveness of various self-concept facets but place global
    self-concept at the top of the hierarchy. Unfortunately, such
    instruments are expensive and generally available only to trained
    psychologists. However, interested readers can find a test of global
    self-concept at: http://literacy.kent.edu/Midwest/Resc/Kansas/psassessment.html. This
    contains (1) a self and tutor rating scale, (2) a checklist for
    identifying difficult daily living situations and (3) a tutor
    observation checklist. Please note, however, that teachers
    without training in counselling should not try to offer therapy
    and that these scales are not designed for
    children.

     Enhancing students'
    self-esteem

     One
    effective way of enhancing students' self-esteem and academic
    achievement is adventure education. In a meta-analysis of
    ninety-six studies of adventure education, Hattie, et al. (1997)
    categorized the benefits of adventure studies into six broad
    outcomes: leadership, self-concept, academic achievement,
    personality, interpersonal relations and adventuresomeness. All
    of the outcomes except adventuresomeness maintained effects over
    time. Positive change is thought to take place because
    participation in problem-solving tasks challenges self-imposed
    limits, leading to improvements in relationships with others and
    self-concept.

    On a day-to-day basis, variations on
    Circle Time have been used at most levels of education for
    enhancing students' general and academic self-esteem by
    challenging limiting beliefs and fostering awareness of multiple
    options. Hillyard (2002), a firm proponent of the metacognitive
    approach in bilingual education in Argentina, claims that not
    only children but also adolescents find whole-class discussions
    of this type highly rewarding.

     Peer
    teaching and peer
    assessment

     Peer
    teaching may involve learners of different ages or of the same
    age. Although not exclusively an experiment in peer teaching, the
    University of Dundee's paired reading project has shown the value
    of support from more able readers (teachers, parents, other
    adults or older children) in developing reading and thinking
    skills among primary school children. Interestingly, it was the
    least able children (both tutors and tutees) that benefited most
    from this activity. The corresponding web page
    http://www.dundee.ac.uk/psychology/ReadOn/ also contains links to
    other articles on peer teaching.

     Group projects are another
    obvious activity for promoting planning, monitoring and
    evaluation through peer teaching, especially among older
    children. Books such as "Project Work" by Diana L. Fried-Booth
    (O.U.P.) – which also contains a project in which adult EFL
    students teach primary school children – provide valuable advice
    and worksheets for teacher, group and individual
    reviews.

     Self-evaluation is a difficult
    strategy to acquire, partly because it often comes at the end of
    a project or task when learners have run out of time, interest or
    both, partly because it often involves comparing oneself with
    others, a strategy recommended by Oxford (1990: 163) but which is
    potentially threatening to learners with low self-esteem.
    Nevertheless, many ELT textbooks contain reading and writing
    activities (e.g. jigsaw reading; assessing other students'
    drafts) in which learners teach one another and receive peer
    feedback on their understanding or performance. A non-threatening
    and on-going method of peer assessment and awareness raising in
    oral skills, which comes with a rationale and materials, can be
    found at http://www.finchpark.com/courses/assess/oralpeer02.htm
    Finally, games can also include informal peer teaching and
    evaluation. One of my own can be found at
    http://www.eslcafe.com/ideas/sefer.cgi?display:989526399-5862.txt

     Conclusion

     Metacognitive
    strategy training enhances learning inside and outside the
    classroom but many students have difficulty in using this
    approach once there is no longer a reminder to do so. Within a
    social-constructivist perspective, metacognitive skills and
    metacognitive knowledge, including a realistic self-concept,
    develop through social interaction and are then internalised. The
    key to more effective metacognitive strategy training would seem
    to be through simultaneous training in social strategies together
    with social learning tasks. The latter may serve to reduce or
    eliminate negative aspects of an individual's self-concept such
    as learned helplessness, negative self-labels, competitiveness,
    perfectionism etc., which prevent realistic and effective
    goal-setting, planning, attending, monitoring or evaluating in
    real life contexts.

      References

    Fried-Booth, D. L. (1986). Project
    Work. Oxford: O.U.P.

    Harrison, C.J. (1991).
    'Metacognition and motivation'. Reading Improvement. Vol. 28. No.
    1 35-38.

    Hattie, J.; Marsh, H. W.; Neill, J.
    T. & Richards, G. E. (1997). Adventure education and Outward
    Bound: Out-of-class experiences that make a lasting difference.
    Review of Educational Research, 67,
    43-87.

    Hillyard, S. (2002). Personal
    Communication (interview regarding on-going investigation at
    Wellspring School, Buenos
    Aires).

    Oxford, R. (1990). Language
    Learning Strategies: What Every Teacher Should Know. Boston MA.:
    Heinle and Heinle Publishers.

    Robbins, J.A. (2002).
    http://jillrobbins.com/articles/LSIrobbins.html (visited Dec.13
    2002)

    Von Wright, J. (1992). 'Reflections
    on reflection'. Learning and Instruction. Vol. 2.
    59-68

    Vygotsky, L.S. (1975). Mind in
    society. The development of higher psychological processes.
    Cambridge MA.: Harvard University
    Press.

    Vygotsky, L.S. (1986). Thought and
    language. Cambridge MA.: MIT
    Press.

     

    Douglas Andrew
    Town
     BSc (Hons) Psychology, MA (English
    Language Teaching), Diploma in
     Translation (Spanish)
     Profesor de la
    Universidad de
    Belgrano, Argentina (Licenciatura en Inglés).
     

    Nota al lector: es posible que esta página no contenga todos los componentes del trabajo original (pies de página, avanzadas formulas matemáticas, esquemas o tablas complejas, etc.). Recuerde que para ver el trabajo en su versión original completa, puede descargarlo desde el menú superior.

    Todos los documentos disponibles en este sitio expresan los puntos de vista de sus respectivos autores y no de Monografias.com. El objetivo de Monografias.com es poner el conocimiento a disposición de toda su comunidad. Queda bajo la responsabilidad de cada lector el eventual uso que se le de a esta información. Asimismo, es obligatoria la cita del autor del contenido y de Monografias.com como fuentes de información.

    Categorias
    Newsletter