Monografias.com > Educación
Descargar Imprimir Comentar Ver trabajos relacionados

What are learning strategies?



     

    1. The Origins of the Term
      "Strategy"
    2. Learner Strategies and Learning
      Strategies
    3. Defining the Term
      "Strategy"
    4. Avoiding
      confusion
    5. Plans or
      processes?
    6. Conclusion
    7. References

    The importance of learning strategies is now widely
    recognized in all areas of education. As Oxford says, "under
    various names such as learning skills, learning-to-learns skills,
    thinking skills, and problem-solving skills, learning strategies
    are the way students learn a wide range of subjects, from native
    language reading through electronics trouble-shooting to new
    languages" (1990:2-3). This article reviews some definitions and
    debates about the nature of learning strategies within the field
    of applied linguistics and ELT.

     The
    Origins of the Term "Strategy"

     The word
    "strategy" comes from the Greek "strategos", a root that
    originally meant "trick" or "deception". The Greeks later used
    the term to describe army generals: a general was one who could
    trick the enemy. The term first became current in English in the
    late 18th and early 19th century when "it denoted the overall
    military and psychological plans that a general made for a
    campaign" (Encyclopaedia Britannica 1985).

     The term was first used in Cognitive Psychology
    in 1956 by Bruner, Goodnow and Austin in a paper presented at a
    meeting of the Massachusetts Institute of Technology. It was an
    auspicious meeting. The paper, which contained the first
    systematic attempt to consider concept formation from a cognitive
    perspective (Eysenck and Keane 1990:7), was presented alongside a
    preliminary paper by Chomsky on his theory of language, George
    Miller's paper on the magic number 7 in short term memory and
    Newell Shaw and Simon's "General Problem Solver", a computational
    model from which later theories relating to problem-solving and
    production systems are derived.

     In Applied Linguistics, strategy research dates
    back to 1966 when Aaron Carton published his study, "The Method
    of Inference in Foreign Language Study". This was followed in the
    mid 1970s by a series of empirical studies of "good" language
    learners, notably by Rubin (1975), Stern (1975) and Naiman,
    Fröhlich, Stern and Todesco (1978). Since the late 1970s
    applied linguistics has turned more and more to cognitive science
    to provide the theoretical framework for language learning and
    this has led to valuable research into a whole range of
    strategies used in vocabulary learning tasks (Cohen and Aphek
    1980, 1981), reading comprehension (Brown et al 1983, Chipman
    Segal and Glaser 1985; Dansereau 1985) and writing (Flower and
    Hayes 1981) to name only a few.

     Learner
    Strategies and Learning Strategies

     A distinction is sometimes made in
    Applied Linguistics between learner strategies and learning
    strategies. Tarone (1981), for example, distinguishes three sets
    of learner strategies: learning strategies, production strategies
    and communication strategies. According to this view, learning
    strategies are the means by which the learner processes the L2
    input to develop linguistic knowledge. Production strategies, on
    the other hand, involve learners' attempts to use L2 knowledge
    they have already acquired efficiently, clearly and with minimum
    effort (in Faerch and Kasper 1983:72-73 and Ellis 1985:13) while
    communication strategies consist of learners' attempts to
    communicate meanings that are beyond their linguistic competence
    by using such devices as paraphrase or gesture.

     While the distinction between learner
    strategies (i.e. any strategies used by learners) and learning
    strategies (i.e. strategies used to process input) is a logical
    one and has been maintained by writers such as Wenden (1987,
    1989) and Skehan (1989), this has not been the case in the United
    States where the term "learning strategy" is used to refer to any
    type of strategy used by learners. This is not a confusing as it
    might appear, however, since writers on both sides of the
    Atlantic (and elsewhere) now recognise that learning can take
    place through communication (Faerch and Kasper 1983: xvii) and
    production, as when a writer is forced to reprocess "old"
    information and language at a deeper level in order to express
    new meanings or more subtle nuances. Strategies that are used to
    manipulate or transform cognitive material are now generally
    known as "cognitive strategies"

     Defining the
    Term "Strategy"

     Nevertheless, strategies are not
    easy to define. As Ellis (1993:9) points out, "there is no
    agreement on exactly what (…) learning strategies are, how many
    of them there are, what they consist of, etc".

     One problem is that the term "strategy" is
    widely used in psychology, education and applied linguistics,
    each of which has its own interests and its own theoretical
    approaches and research methodology. These differences, although
    they should not be exaggerated, have been notable in the past. In
    applied linguistics, for example, earlier definitions of
    strategies tended to stress their
    behavioural aspects simply because much research at that time was
    based on observation of what good language learners did to learn
    a language, whereas psychology took a more "mentalist" approach.
    Thus, Rubin (1975:43) originally defines strategies as
    "techniques or devices which a learner may use to acquire
    knowledge" and twelve years later she still stressed "what
    learners do to learn" as well as "what learners do to regulate
    their learning" (Rubin and Wenden 1987:19. italics in
    original).

     By contrast, Gagné (1977:35) sees
    strategies as "skills by means of which learners regulate their
    own internal processes of attending, learning, remembering and
    thinking", and more recently Best (1986:463) writes: "Strategies
    are seen in behaviour, but the behaviour implies some sort mental
    effort. A strategy can therefore be defined as a move, trial or
    probe designed to effect some change in a problem and provide
    information by doing so." Best divides strategies into two broad
    classes: heuristics and algorithms, which are described in the
    psychology literature in connection with
    problem-solving.

     These differences are nevertheless questions of
    emphasis rather than fundamental disagreements and the same is
    true of distinctions made within particular
    disciplines.

    In education, for example, a "strategic approach" has
    been contrasted with a "deep approach" and a "surface approach"
    (Entwistle 1987:60). What was most distinctive about the
    strategic approach was the use of well-planned and carefully
    organised study methods or "study strategies". More recently,
    however, study skills or strategies have been introduced within a
    more general framework that emphasises "deep strategic
    approaches" (Entwistle 1987:69).

     Similarly, differences in emphasis (admittedly
    more subtle ones) are to be found in applied linguistics among
    writers who have turned to information processing models for a
    theoretical framework in which to describe learning strategies.
    Rubin (in Wenden and Rubin 1987:19) following O'Malley et al
    (1983) defines learning strategies as "any set of operations,
    steps plans routines used by the learner to facilitate the
    obtaining, storage retrieval and use of information". This
    definition, while excellent as far as it goes, seems to be based
    partly on the structural multi-store model of memory and says
    nothing about levels or depth of processing. Significantly,
    although Wenden and Rubin mention learning style in passing
    (1987:22), this concept-, which includes "deep" and "shallow"
    approaches-, is not developed in their work.

     O'Malley and Chamot (1990), on the other hand,
    take a more process-based view derived from Anderson's
    (1983-1985) ACT* cognitive architecture. For O'Malley and Chamot,
    learning strategies are "special ways of processing information
    that enhance comprehension, learning or retention of the
    information" (1990:1), and while advocating that certain
    strategies should be taught to all students, they recognise (at
    least implicitly) that different learners prefer to process
    information at different levels. For example, "a visual learner
    may naturally use imagery as a preferred strategy, and a
    field-independent or analytic learner may naturally gravitate
    toward strategies such as grouping and deduction"
    (1990:163).

     Avoiding
    confusion

     When we learn a new concept, we
    need to know which attributes are relevant and which irrelevant.
    We also need to know in what way the new concept is similar to or
    differs from other concepts and whether these are related
    hierarchically or not.

     Irrelevant attributes may lead to definitions
    that are either too broad or too narrow. One definition which is
    too broad, I think, is that offered by Wenden (in Wenden and
    Rubin 1987:6-7), who claims that learner strategies refer not
    only to learner behaviours but also "to what learners know about
    the strategies they use" and "what learners know about aspects of
    their (…) learning other than the strategies they use". While
    such knowledge is invaluable for effective strategy training (see
    Oxford 1990:12), and may lead to learners discovering new
    strategies unassisted, the proof of the pudding is surely in the
    eating.

     By contrast, Seliger's (1984) distinction
    between strategies and tactics makes the concept of strategy too
    narrow. Seliger claims that strategies are "basic abstract
    categories of processing" in contrast to tactics, which "evolve
    to meet the demands of the moment or fluctuate more slowly…"
    (1984:41). This distinction recalls Gagné's (1977:36)
    claim that "cognitive strategies are largely independent of
    content, and generally apply to all kinds (of content)". But as
    Gagné himself recognises, "these mental operations must
    have something to work on – they cannot be exercised in a vacuum"
    (1977:37).

     Seliger's distinction would only be meaningful
    if strategies were innate and tactics were learned (which he does
    not say) since all strategies must begin by meeting the demands
    of some moment or other, whether or not they are later
    generalised to other context. If, as Harlow (1959) claims,
    strategies consist of a general skill or a simple rule or code
    (Gross 1992:196), then it is likely that strategies become
    generalised in much the same way as skills through "tuning". (See
    also O'Malley and Chamot 1990:43).

     Plans or
    processes?

     Another problem that arises when
    defining strategies is whether to consider them as a process or a
    product of learning or both.

     Both Faerch and Kasper (1983) and Ellis (1985)
    make a distinction between strategies and processes. Ellis
    (1985:166) defines strategies as "plans for controlling the other
    in which a sequence of operations is to be performed" while
    processes are "operations involved in the development or
    realisation of a plan".

     In this sense, processes are subordinate to
    strategies. Faerch and Kasper (1983:29), on the other hand, point
    out that among other possible explanations, the term strategy may
    refer to "a specific subclass of processes". My own view is that
    it is not possible to separate the plan from the process
    (otherwise strategies cannot be described in behavioural terms
    either). In this sense, I would agree with Faerch and Kasper in
    considering strategies to be special kinds of
    processes.

     Conclusion

     The literature on learning
    strategies is confusing because, in the past, psychology,
    education and applied linguistics had quite different research
    agendas. In applied linguistics, the move from describing
    strategies in terms of behaviour to explaining them in terms of
    underlying mental processes reflects the abandoning of
    behaviourism as a general theory of learning in favour of models
    drawn from cognitive psychology. However, the problems of
    deciding whether strategies as universally valid procedures or a
    reflection of individual learning style, or whether they are best
    considered as generalised skills as distinct from responses to
    concrete situations, are difficult to grasp without
    understanding, too, how theories of memory and problem-solving
    have evolved over the years.

     The broader challenge is understand how
    learning strategies interact with the learner's existing
    communicative competence in order to enhance learning. O'Malley
    and Chamot's adoption of Anderson's ACT* cognitive architecture
    (which may, itself, soon be superseded by connectivist models)
    unwittingly challenged the notion of language as a discrete set
    of competences, among which strategic competence originally
    played a relatively minor role, suggesting, as many psychologists
    already believed, that language is a skill like any other and
    that language learning is parasitic upon other more general
    cognitive processes.

    In a sense, the wheel has come full circle in applied
    linguistics: Behaviourism was atheorethical in that it was not
    interested in mental processes; by failing to make explicit the
    theoretical framework on which they based their description of
    learning strategies, O'Malley and Chamot and Oxford simply
    exchanged one set of recipes for another.

    But teachers always need to be clear about the
    theoretical underpinnings of what they teach in the classroom,
    don't they?

      REFERENCES

     Best, J.B. (1986). Cognitive
    Psychology. 2nd ed. St. Paul, Mn.: West Publishing
    Company.

    Ellis, R. (1985). Understanding second language
    acquisition. Oxford: OUP.

    Ellis, R. (1993). 'Second language acquisition
    research: How does it help teachers? An interview with Rod
    Ellis'. ELT Journal. Jan. 1993.

    Encyclopaedia Britannica. (1985).

    Entwistle, N. (1988). Understanding classroom
    learning. London: Hodder and Stoughton.

    Eysenck, N.W. and Keane, M.T. (1990). Cognitive
    psychology: A student's handbook. Howe, East Sussex:
    Erlbaum.

    Faerch, C. and Kasper, G. (1983). Strategies in
    interlanguage communication. London: Longman.

    Gagné, R.M. (1977). The conditions of
    learning. 3rd. ed. New York: Holt, Rinehart and
    Winston.

    Gross, R.D. (1992). Psychology: The science of mind
    and behaviour. London: Hodder and Stoughton.

    Naiman, N. et al. (1978). The good language leaner.
    Research and Education Series, 7. Ontario Institute for Study and
    Education.

    O'Malley, J.M. and Chamot, A.U. (1990). Learning
    Strategies in Second Language Acquisition. Cambridge:
    CUP

    Oxford, R. (1990). Language learning strategies.
    Boston, Mass.: Heinle and Heinle.

     Palincsar, A.S. and Brown, L.A. (1986).
    Interactive teaching to promote independent learning from text.
    Reading Teacher. 32 (8). 771-777.

    Rubin, J. (1975). What the good language learner can
    teach us. TESOL Quarterly 9. 41-51.

    Rubin, J. (1981). Study of cognitive processes in
    second language learning. Applied Linguistics.
    117-131.

    Seliger H. (1984) 'Processing universals in SLA'.
    Universals in Second Language Acquisition ed. by Eckmann F. et
    al

    Skehan, P. (1989). Individual differences in second
    language learning. London: Arnold.

    Wenden, A. (1989). Learner strategies for learner
    autonomy. London: Prentice Hall.

    Wenden, A. and Rubin, J. (1987). Learner strategies
    in language learning. London: Prentice Hall.

      

    Douglas Andrew Town
     BSc
    (Hons) Psychology, MA (English Language Teaching), Diploma in
     Translation (Spanish)
     Profesor de la
    Universidad de
    Belgrano, Argentina (Licenciatura en Inglés).
     

    Nota al lector: es posible que esta página no contenga todos los componentes del trabajo original (pies de página, avanzadas formulas matemáticas, esquemas o tablas complejas, etc.). Recuerde que para ver el trabajo en su versión original completa, puede descargarlo desde el menú superior.

    Todos los documentos disponibles en este sitio expresan los puntos de vista de sus respectivos autores y no de Monografias.com. El objetivo de Monografias.com es poner el conocimiento a disposición de toda su comunidad. Queda bajo la responsabilidad de cada lector el eventual uso que se le de a esta información. Asimismo, es obligatoria la cita del autor del contenido y de Monografias.com como fuentes de información.

    Categorias
    Newsletter